Skip to main content

Enhanced pain facilitation rather than impaired pain inhibition in burning mouth syndrome female patients

Abstract

Background

Deficient endogenous pain modulation has been implicated in the development and exacerbation of chronic orofacial pain. To date, relatively little is known regarding the function of the endogenous pain modulation in patients with burning mouth syndrome (BMS). This case–control study investigated endogenous pain modulation in women with BMS.

Methods

Conditioned pain modulation (CPM) was assessed upon temporal summation (TSP) of thermal pain. Forty female subjects, 20 BMS patients and 20 age-matched control subjects, were included in a 2 session-protocol. Mechanical and thermal pain thresholds were measured on the forearm and hand. TSP was obtained using repetitive laser-evoked thermal stimuli applied on the non-dominant hand, at an intensity yielding to moderate pain. During TSP, CPM was produced by immersing the contralateral foot in a water bath at painful cold (8 °C) temperature. In control conditions, the foot was immersed in a water bath at not painful (30 °C) temperature.

Results

BMS was not associated with any impairment in thermal as well as mechanical extracephalic pain thresholds. TSP and CPM efficacy were similar in BMS patients and control subjects. However, BMS patients exhibited enhanced extracephalic heat hyperalgesia.

Conclusion

This study reveals that there is no impairment of endogenous pain inhibition mechanisms in BMS patients, but rather an increase in pain facilitation.

Peer Review reports

Key findings

  • • BMS was not associated with any impairment in thermal as well as mechanical extracephalic pain thresholds.

  • • TSP and CPM efficacy were similar in BMS patients and control subjects.

  • • BMS patients exhibited enhanced extracephalic heat hyperalgesia

Background

Burning mouth syndrome (BMS) is a chronic and spontaneously non-remitting oral pain without any identifiable local anatomical lesion or laboratory finding [1]. The pathogenesis and etiology of BMS are still unknown. Although it was initially classified as a psychalgic pain [2], recent evidence suggests that BMS is rather a neuropathic disorder [1]. However, the very cause of the neuropathic changes is still unclear. Combined dysregulations of adrenal, gonadal and neuroactive steroids, dysfunction of gustatory and somatic afferents have been suggested to contribute to the pathogenesis of BMS [1]. Interestingly, BMS often coexists with other syndromes referred to as central sensitivity syndromes [3] such as temporo-mandibular disorder (TMD), fibromyalgia or visceral pain [4, 5]. BMS might thus share common mechanisms with these chronic pain syndromes, including abnormal endogenous pain modulation [6, 7]. One of the major endogenous pain inhibitory systems is conditioned pain modulation (CPM; formerly diffuse noxious inhibitory controls), a powerful general endogenous analgesic mechanism which can completely inhibit incoming nociceptor signals at the primary synapse [8]. CPM is believed to play an important role in the development and exacerbation of chronic pain, because dysfunction of CPM is associated with a shift in balance between pain facilitation and pain inhibition. In many (but not all) patients with central sensitization, CPM is less efficacious [9]. Although an association between deficient CPM and the development of BMS was found in a previous study [10], here, we reassessed CPM in BMS patients using a brand new experimental protocol [11]. The primary endpoint of this exploratory case–control study was to evidence any alteration of CPM in BMS patients.

Methods

Study design

This is a case–control study conducted in the Clinical Investigation Centre of Clermont-Ferrand University Hospital (France). The study conformed to the STROBE guidelines for case–control studies. The study was performed after approval was obtained from the referent Ethics Committee “CCP Sud-Est VI”. Standard written informed consent was obtained from all participants, according to the 1964 declaration of Helsinki. The study was registered as N°2010-A00403-36.

Participants

The sample size calculation was based on pilot data obtained in eight healthy subjects from the Clermont-Ferrand center, in which CPM decreased the test heat pain by 2.6 ± 1.8. Expecting that the CPM in BMS patients would be decreased compared to controls as it is the case in fibromyalgia patients [12], the sample size was calculated to allow for the identification of a difference of 1.7 points (65% reduction) with a standard deviation of 1.8. With a statistical power of 80% and a two-tailed type I error of 5%, 18 patients per group were found to be necessary. We decided to include 20 subjects per group to increase the power of the study.

The patients were recruited from the Clermont-Ferrand Hospital Orofacial Pain Clinic. Identification of the patients was carried out taking into account the inclusion/exclusion criteria reported below.

Inclusion criteria

BMS group: females patient over 40 year-old; daily presence of localized burning sensation in the oral mucosa during all or part of the day lasting for longer than 3 months; no paroxysm and not following any unilateral nerve trajectory; no clinical oral mucosal alterations; pain intensity ≥ 3 on a Visual Analogue Scale (VAS).

For all participants: subjects must have given their written consent.

Exclusion criteria

BMS group: report of pain and/or burning in the mouth of known origin (cancer, infectious, traumatic, iron deficiency anemia, diabetes); recent change in current medication. Unknown local or systemic pathologies were ruled out thanks to the medical history and blood sampling looking for iron deficiencies, vitamins, diabetes or thyroid dysfunction.

For all participants: taking one of the following antidepressants: venlafaxine, duloxetine, tricyclic antidepressants or bupropion; current medication with neuroleptics; co-morbidity with another ongoing chronic pain (e.g. fibromyalgia, chronic low back pain, chronic pelvic pain, tension headache, migraine, irritable bowel syndrome, temporomandibular disorder); progressive cardiovascular disease; subject not cooperating or unable to speak or read French fluently, or to understand a pain scale; major cognitive or incapacitating disorder.

The control group consisted of 20 females, matched for age, who were recruited among patients attending the Oral Medicine Clinic for identifiable organic causes such as decayed tooth, dental treatment and other mucosal diseases. Only women were included in the study, since BMS is mostly a woman’s pathology and CPM display sex specific features [6].

Questionnaires

Standard demographic information, duration of complaint, health status, medical use and pain medications used before the examination were collected. At the same time, all subjects also completed a series of self-report questionnaires including the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) short form, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) and the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS).

Pain intensity ratings

The burning pain intensity in the past week was rated using both the 0–10 cm visual analogue scale (VAS) and the 4-point-(none, mild, moderate and severe)-verbal rating scale (VRS).

Pain threshold measurements

Explorations were conducted in quiet conditions and at constant temperature (23 °C). For measurement of mechanical pain thresholds (MPTs), punctuate stimuli were applied on the forearm using an electronic algometer (electronic von Frey, Bioseb, France). The strain gauge was connected to a plastic sterile cone (Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany), the tip of which was applied perpendicularly to the studied skin area. The punctate pressure was gradually increased with a constant slope under visual control of the pressure value up to the detection of MPT. Threshold was defined as the lowest pressure that produced a sensation of pain. The results of three separate consecutive measurements at different points in the testing area were averaged to establish the MPT value. The applied pressure could not exceed 500 g, which was the default threshold if pain was not elicited at this level.

Measurement of heat pain and tolerance thresholds were performed using a thermotester (SENSELab®, Somedic, Sweden) on the skin of the non-dominant forearm. The dimensions of the skin thermode were 25 × 50 mm. The baseline temperature was set at 32 °C, the maximum temperature was 52 °C and the temperature change rate was set at 0.5 and 1.0 °C.s–1, respectively for pain and tolerance threshold measurement. The patient pushed a stop-button when the threshold was reached and there was an interval of five seconds between repeated thermal stimuli. The whole procedure was repeated five times and the average of five thresholds was taken. In pre-experimental explanations given to patients, particular emphasis was placed on the concept of pain threshold as opposed to tolerance threshold, in order to avoid bias due to patient fear. Immediate pain intensity was scored by the patient on a 0–10 cm visual analogue scale (VAS).

Conditioned pain modulation measurements

To explore the subjects’ individual CPM potency, the temporal summation of pain (TSP) evoked by a stimulation of the back of the non-dominant hand by a YAP laser stimulator (fiber-optic guidance, diameter 5 mm, duration 2 ms) was used as test stimulus. A stimulator with optic-fiber guidance was placed on a skin area of about 6 cm2 at the dorsal aspect of the hand. Cutaneous heat stimuli were applied in incremental intensities in steps of 0.25 J, starting at a minimum of 0.75 J and reaching a maximum of 2 J. After determination of the pain threshold, the laser energy that was able to produce a pain intensity between 3–6 on 0–10 VAS was determined (~ 10 min before CPM assessment). On the one hand, such intensity was needed to elicit sufficient pain to observe a CPM effect, but, on the other hand, not too strong, for ethical reasons. Thereafter, the stimulation at this same intensity was applied repeatedly (from 1st to 45th) every 40 s for 30 min. Thus, each patient received a series of 45 stimulations applied at constant intensity. Subjects were asked to score pain on a 0–10 VAS scale after each block of stimulation. The laser beam was moved (~ 1 cm) between each block to avoid skin lesions.

The conditioning stimulus consisted in the immersion of the contralateral foot into a water bath at noxious cold temperature (8 °C), between the 10th and 20th min of the experiment. In control experiments that were performed on a different day in the same subjects, the water bath temperature was neutral (30 °C). In all cases, water was constantly re-circulated to prevent laminar warming around the immersed foot. All subjects were asked to rate the cold pain on a 0–10 VAS scale immediately at the end of immersion. The order CPM vs. control experiments was assigned randomly.

The responses for each block in each condition were plotted over time. The first 15 responses were used to study the unconditioned responses (1st to 15th), the next 15 ones (16th to 30th) to study CPM and the 15 last ones (31st to 45th) to study the post-effects of CPM.

Statistical analysis

Statistics were computed with STATA V12 (Stata Corp, College Station, TX, USA). Results were expressed as mean ± standard deviation or median (interquartile range) and as frequencies (percentage). Groups were compared using Chi square test (or Fisher’s exact test when appropriate) for categorical data, and using Student t-test (or Mann–Whitney when data not normal) test depending on data distribution for continuous data. Normality was assessed graphically and using Shapiro–Wilk's test. Relationships between the pain intensity and BDI, STAI, and PCS scores were assessed by Pearson’s correlation coefficient (or Spearman’s when data were not normal). Intra-groups comparisons of pain at discrete time points were performed using Students paired t-test.

Pain perception was analyzed using linear mixed models with random subject intercept. Groups, time and foot water immersion were tested as fixed effects. Those methods were developed for temporal subdivision (control, water immersion and post-water immersion periods) in order to test: 1) the session effect, 2) the effect of the repeated painful stimulation, 3) the water immersion effect and the post-water immersion effect. Those analyzes were repeated intra and inter-groups. Moreover, for the 1) and 2) we adjusted for the mean pain of the 15 first blocks (= baseline adjustment). All tests were two-sided and a P value < 5% was considered statistically significant.

Results

Study population

The control participants were age- and gender-matched with the BMS patients. The clinical characteristics of the BMS patients are displayed in Table 1. No differences were found between groups, except that BMS patients were more anxious than controls. The depression scores of all participants were within the normal range. The mechanical, heat and laser pain thresholds did not differ between BMS patients and healthy controls (Table 2).

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristic of BMS patients and control subjects
Table 2 Mechanical and thermal pain thresholds and pain rating

Temporal summation of pain

Following repetitive laser-mediated thermal stimuli, pain intensity progressively increased in both groups, showing TSP (Fig. 1) Pain intensity increased from the first to the 15th block of stimulation in control subjects (paired t test, t = 4.94, df = 39, P < 0.001), from 26.7 ± 2.7 to 46.5 ± 4.8, as well as in BMS patients (paired student t-test, t = 3.45, df = 39, P = 0.001), from 38.1 ± 2.6 to 51.7 ± 3.1. Moreover, while the magnitude of TSP – as calculated by the difference between the last and the first pain rating – did not differ between BMS patients and control subjects (Student's t-test, t = 1.11, df = 78, P = 0.269), the sum of pain intensities between the 1st and the 15th block of stimuli was greater in BMS patients than control subjects (Fig. 1). Thus, BMS patients demonstrated significantly greater heat hyperalgesia than control subjects (Linear mixed model: P = 0.012 for group, P < 0.001 for time, P = 0.514 for time × group.), while there was no difference in TSP.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Temporal summation of pain in burning mouth syndrome (BMS) patients and control subjects. A Time course of pain perception evoked by repetitive laser painful stimuli applied of the hand in control subjects, and BMS patients. Testing sessions consisted of a series of 15 blocks of 4 thermal test stimuli (Nd:YAP laser stimulator) that were delivered at 0.2 Hz on the back of the non-dominant hand, repeated every 40 s for 10 min. Mean pain ratings (± SEM) are shown from the first to the 15th stimulation. *Linear mixed model: P = 0.012 for group, P < 0.001 for time, P = 0.514 for time × group. B Sum of pain intensity between 1st-15th.. *P value is from two-tailed Student's unpaired t-test

As BMS patients exhibited higher STAI scores than control subjects, we tested whether the severities of anxiety, on the one hand, and pain (between the 1st and 15th block of stimuli), on the other hand, are correlated. There was no correlation between STAI scores and pain intensities in the control subjects (chi2 = 0.02, df = 1, p = 0.893), as well as BMS patients (chi2 = 1.51, df = 1, p = 0.219). We also tested whether STAI scores (which are higher in BMS patients than in control subjects) might affect the group effect, by assessing the group x STAI interaction. We found no significant interaction (chi2 = 0.56, df = 1, p = 0.456).

Conditioned pain modulation

In the present study, we performed two analyses. First, we assessed the intensity of pain evoked by the laser stimulation before (1st-15th stimulations), during (effect, 16th-30th stimulations) and after (post-effect, 31st -45th stimulations) immersing the contralateral foot into a water bath at painful cold (8 °C) temperature. Second, we also compared the intensity of laser stimulation-evoked pain during (effect, 16th-30th stimulations) and after (post-effect, 31st-45th stimulations) application of the painful conditioning stimulation (water at 8 °C, 16th-30th stimulations) with those during and after application of a non-painful conditioning stimulation (water at 30 °C).

In control subjects, laser-evoked pain was depressed during (time effect, chi2 = 119, df = 14, P < 0.001) and after (time effect, chi2 = 58.7, df = 14, P < 0.001) the application of the painful conditioning stimulation on the contralateral foot (Fig. 2A). Compared to the non-painful conditioning stimulation, the magnitude of CPM was higher during (cold-water effect, chi2 = 29, df = 1, P < 0.001) and after (cold-water post-effect, chi2 = 20, df = 1, P < 0.001) the application of the painful conditioning stimulation of the contralateral foot.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Time course of pain perception evoked by repetitive painful stimuli of the hand before (1st to 15th), during (16th to 30th), and after (31st to 45th) application of non-painful (in yellow) or painful (in green) conditioning thermal stimulation on the contralateral foot in (A) control subjects and (B) burning mouth syndrome patients. To assess CPM, blocks of 4 thermal test stimuli (Nd:YAP laser stimulator) were delivered at 0.2 Hz on the back of the non-dominant hand, repeated every 40 s for 30 min, at an intensity producing a pain intensity between 3–6 on 0–10 VAS. Between the 10th and 20th min (16th to 30th), the contralateral foot was immersed into a water bath at non-painful (30 °C) or painful cold (8 °C) temperature. A linear mixed model was used to explore the effects of CPM. P values indicate the differences between the non-painful and painful conditioning thermal stimulation

In BMS patients, laser-evoked pain was also depressed during (time effect, chi2 = 34.1, df = 14, P = 0.002) and after (time effect, chi2 = 42.6, df = 14, P < 0.001) the application of the painful conditioning stimulation on the contralateral foot (Fig. 2B). Compared to non-painful conditioning stimulation, the magnitude of CPM was higher during (cold-water effect, chi2 = 19.5, df = 1, P < 0.001) and after (cold water post-effect, chi2 = 9.47, df = 1, P = 0.002) the conditioning stimulation. However, there was no group effect (patients vs. controls) on the CPM, either during (interaction group x cold-water effect, chi2 = 0.04, df = 1, P = 0.837) or after (interaction group x cold water effect, chi2 = 0.73, df = 1, P = 0.392) the conditioning stimulation.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated whether impaired endogenous pain modulation could be part of the mechanisms underlying BMS in women. We found no changes in CPM efficiency in BMS patients, no changes in TSP, while neither thermal nor mechanical extracephalic pain thresholds were altered. However, the sensory profile of BMS patients was characterized by enhanced extracephalic heat hyperalgesia.

CPM is a powerful endogenous analgesic mechanism whereby a nociceptive stimulation applied to a given body location reduces the percept and brain responses elicited by noxious (test) stimuli delivered at a remote body location [6, 8]. Dysfunction of CPM is assumed to shift the balance between pain facilitation and inhibition toward facilitation and the development of chronic pain [6]. Here, we tested the hypothesis that abnormal CPM may be part of the pain mechanisms in BMS. However, in contrast to a previous study [10], this was not the case, since BMS patients were able to normally recruit their endogenous pain inhibitory system in our experimental setting. A similar hypothesis has been raised regarding the pathophysiology of other chronic orofacial pain conditions, but CPM has not always been reported to be impaired [13, 14] and appeared normal in some studies [7, 15,16,17] Such contradictory results – due in part to differences in methods used to assess CPM [6]—and the lack of aggregate analysis of the literature prevent any solid conclusion about the function of endogenous pain modulation in patients with chronic orofacial pain [7].

TSP, when assessed in response to repetitive laser-evoked thermal stimuli [11], was similar in BMS patients and control subjects. Similarly, a previous study, using intradermal electrical stimulation of the chin, found no difference in TPS between BMS and controls [10]. This is consistent with previous findings in other chronic orofacial pain syndromes, such as TMD, where TSP was normal when assessed with thermal stimuli [18,19,20,21,22]. In such patients though, TSP could be found altered [17, 23], when assessed with mechanical stimuli, as it sometimes increased [20]. Since thermal TSP relies, at least in part, on the activation of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, and thermal TSP is not altered in BMS patients, it seems likely that NMDA receptors do not play a critical role in the pathophysiology of BMS. Similar mechanistic suggestions have been made about patients suffering from persistent post-endodontic pain [14].

Despite normal TSP, BMS patients showed greater extracephalic heat hyperalgesia than control subjects. An enhanced heat hyperalgesia seems common in patients with chronic, either cephalic [20, 21] or extracephalic [24], pain. In all these conditions, the presence of hyperalgesia suggests a facilitation of pain mechanisms in BMS patients. In addition, hyperalgesia has been shown to be predictive of poor outcomes in some chronic pain conditions [25], and this may also apply to BMS.

Although we cannot exclude the possibility that the enhanced heat hyperalgesia in BMS patients results from peripheral mechanisms, several observations rather suggest a central contribution. First, neither heat pain thresholds nor MPTs were reduced in the BMS group, showing that thermal and mechanical nociceptors were not sensitized. Second, that That hyperalgesia was observed in a healthy area (hand), remotely located from the painful site (mouth), suggests that it is a secondary hyperalgesia. There is ample evidence for secondary hyperalgesia resulting, at least in part, from a facilitation of nociceptive transmission – i.e. central sensitization – within the dorsal horn [26]. Finally, since CPM efficiency was similar in BMS patients and controls subjects, a reduction in CPM cannot explain the enhanced heat hyperalgesia that may rather relies on increased pain facilitating mechanisms.

Prevalence of psychological disorders, anxiety especially, is high in BMS patients, but its role in the pathogenesis of BMS remains unclear (1). Psychological assessment using the STAI reported significantly higher scores in the BMS patients compared with control subjects. However, no correlation between STAI score and pain ratings could be found. Previous studies, too, saw no association between pain ratings and psychological factors in BMS patients [27, 28]. Taken together, these results suggest that the contribution of anxiety to pain rating differences is limited.

Limitations

We are aware of the limitations that apply to the present study. First, our sample was limited to females and our results thus cannot be generalized to males. Second, we studied a peri-menopausal population in which CPM effect is known to weaken with age [29]. Third, although the sample size was selected based on a previous study [12], the methodology in the present study did not exactly match that in the previous one, and consequently, the sample size might have been underestimated to detect between‐group differences. Finally, we did not look for possible differences between body regions [30].

Conclusions

This study reveals that BMS patients’ sensory profile is characterized by enhanced extracephalic heat hyperalgesia, without alterations in CPM. Altogether, the results suggest that an increase in endogenous pain facilitation and no impairment in inhibition underlies BMS in women.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BDI:

Beck depression inventory

BMS:

Burning mouth syndrome

CPM:

Conditioned pain modulation

HPT:

Heat pain thresholds

MPT:

Mechanical pain threshold

PCS:

Pain catastrophizing scale

STAI:

State-trait anxiety inventory

TSP:

Temporal summation of pain

VAS:

Visual analogue scale

VRS:

Verbal rating scale

References

  1. Jääskeläinen SK, Woda A (2017) Burning mouth syndrome. Cephalalgia Int J Headache 37:627–647. https://doi.org/10.1177/0333102417694883

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Eli I, Kleinhauz M, Baht R, Littner M (1994) Antecedents of Burning Mouth Syndrome (Glossodynia)-Recent Life Events vs. Psychopathologic Aspects J Dent Res 73:567–572. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220345940730021301

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Yunus MB (2008) Central Sensitivity Syndromes: A New Paradigm and Group Nosology for Fibromyalgia and Overlapping Conditions, and the Related Issue of Disease versus Illness. Semin Arthritis Rheum 37:339–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.semarthrit.2007.09.003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Mignogna MD, Pollio A, Fortuna G et al (2011) Unexplained somatic comorbidities in patients with burning mouth syndrome: a controlled clinical study. J Orofac Pain 25:131–140

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Moisset X, Calbacho V, Torres P et al (2016) Co-occurrence of Pain Symptoms and Somatosensory Sensitivity in Burning Mouth Syndrome: A Systematic Review. PLoS One 11(9):e0163449. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163449

  6. van Wijk G, Veldhuijzen DS (2010) Perspective on diffuse noxious inhibitory controls as a model of endogenous pain modulation in clinical pain syndromes. J Pain 11:408–419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2009.10.009

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Moana-Filho EJ, Herrero Babiloni A, Theis-Mahon NR (2018) Endogenous pain modulation in chronic orofacial pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 159:1441–1455. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001263

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Le Bars D (2002) The whole body receptive field of dorsal horn multireceptive neurones. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 40:29–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0173(02)00186-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Yarnitsky D (2010) Conditioned pain modulation (the diffuse noxious inhibitory control-like effect): its relevance for acute and chronic pain states. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol 23:611–615. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACO.0b013e32833c348b

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Nishihara C, Watanabe K, Ozasa K et al (2020) Altered pain modulation to noxious heat thermal stimuli in burning mouth syndrome. Oral Dis 26:1777–1782. https://doi.org/10.1111/odi.13486

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Guy N, Voisin D, Mulliez A et al (2018) Medication overuse reinstates conditioned pain modulation in women with migraine. Cephalalgia Int J Headache 38:1148–1158. https://doi.org/10.1177/0333102417727545

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Potvin S, Larouche A, Normand E et al (2010) No relationship between the ins del polymorphism of the serotonin transporter promoter and pain perception in fibromyalgia patients and healthy controls. Eur J Pain Lond Engl 14:742–746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.12.004

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. King CD, Wong F, Currie T et al (2009) Deficiency in endogenous modulation of prolonged heat pain in patients with Irritable Bowel Syndrome and Temporomandibular Disorder. Pain 143:172–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2008.12.027

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Nasri-Heir C, Khan J, Benoliel R et al (2015) Altered pain modulation in patients with persistent postendodontic pain. Pain 156:2032–2041. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000265

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Baad-Hansen L, List T, Kaube H et al (2006) Blink reflexes in patients with atypical odontalgia and matched healthy controls. Exp Brain Res 172:498–506. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0358-1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kothari SF, Baad-Hansen L, Oono Y, Svensson P (2015) Somatosensory assessment and conditioned pain modulation in temporomandibular disorders pain patients. Pain 156:2545–2555. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000325

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Garrett PH, Sarlani E, Grace EG, Greenspan JD (2013) Chronic temporomandibular disorders are not necessarily associated with a compromised endogenous analgesic system. J Orofac Pain 27:142–150. https://doi.org/10.11607/jop.943

  18. Maixner W, Fillingim R, Sigurdsson A et al (1998) Sensitivity of patients with painful temporomandibular disorders to experimentally evoked pain: evidence for altered temporal summation of pain. Pain 76:71–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00028-1

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Raphael KG, Janal MN, Anathan S et al (2009) Temporal summation of heat pain in temporomandibular disorder patients. J Orofac Pain 23:54–64

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Greenspan JD, Slade GD, Bair E et al (2011) Pain sensitivity risk factors for chronic TMD: descriptive data and empirically identified domains from the OPPERA case control study. J Pain 12:T61-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2011.08.006

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Ribeiro-Dasilva MC, Goodin BR, Fillingim RB (2012) Differences in suprathreshold heat pain responses and self-reported sleep quality between patients with temporomandibular joint disorder and healthy controls. Eur J Pain Lond Engl 16:983–993. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2011.00108.x

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Sato H, Saisu H, Muraoka W et al (2012) Lack of temporal summation but distinct aftersensations to thermal stimulation in patients with combined tension-type headache and myofascial temporomandibular disorder. J Orofac Pain 26:288–295

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Pfau DB, Rolke R, Nickel R et al (2009) Somatosensory profiles in subgroups of patients with myogenic temporomandibular disorders and Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Pain 147:72–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.08.010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Staud R, Weyl EE, Price DD, Robinson ME (2012) Mechanical and heat hyperalgesia highly predict clinical pain intensity in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes. J Pain 13:725–735. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.04.006

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Coombes BK, Bisset L, Vicenzino B (2012) Thermal hyperalgesia distinguishes those with severe pain and disability in unilateral lateral epicondylalgia. Clin J Pain 28:595–601. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e31823dd333

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Latremoliere A, Woolf CJ (2009) Central sensitization: a generator of pain hypersensitivity by central neural plasticity. J Pain 10:895–926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.012

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Honda M, Iida T, Kamiyama H et al (2019) Mechanical sensitivity and psychological factors in patients with burning mouth syndrome. Clin Oral Investig 23:757–762. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00784-018-2488-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Yang G, Su S, Jie H et al (2019) Somatosensory Profiling of Patients with Burning Mouth Syndrome and Correlations with Psychologic Factors J Oral Facial. Pain Headache 33:278–286. https://doi.org/10.11607/ofph.2358

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hackett J, Naugle KE, Naugle KM (2020) The Decline of Endogenous Pain Modulation With Aging: A Meta-Analysis of Temporal Summation and Conditioned Pain Modulation. J Pain 21:514–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2019.09.005

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Levy D, Abdian L, Dekel-Steinkeller M, Defrin R (2018) Experimental evidence for weaker endogenous inhibition of trigeminal pain than extra-trigeminal pain in healthy individuals. Cephalalgia Int J Headache 38:1307–1315. https://doi.org/10.1177/0333102417735851

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Pr A Artola and Pr D Voisin for helpful comments on this manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by funding from Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (Inserm), Université Clermont Auvergne (France), CHU Clermont-Ferrand (France) and “APICIL" Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Study concept and design: CG, PP, CD, RD. Acquisition of data: CG, PP. Analysis and interpretation of data: CG, PP, AM, CD, RD. Drafting and critical revision of the manuscript: CG, PP, AM, CD, RD. All The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Radhouane Dallel.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the French ethics committee “CCP Sud-Est VI”. The study was registered as N°2010-A00403-36. Detailed information about the experiment was given to all participants and informed written consent was obtained. All aspects of the study were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gremeau-Richard, C., Pionchon, P., Mulliez, A. et al. Enhanced pain facilitation rather than impaired pain inhibition in burning mouth syndrome female patients. J Headache Pain 23, 143 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s10194-022-01516-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s10194-022-01516-7

Keywords