Skip to main content

Preventive treatment can reverse cognitive impairment in chronic migraine

Abstract

Objective

To study the impact of chronic migraine (CM) on the cognition and quality of life (QoL) of patients in the interictal period, and to analyse the degree of reversibility of any observed alterations following the use of preventive treatment.

Background

CM is a highly disabling disease, and migraineurs often have associated comorbidities, such as subjective memory problems, that are involved in the development of cognitive impairment. Our hypotheses are that patients suffering from chronic migraine experience objective cognitive alterations that are not only due to the pain that they suffer or their current emotional state. Furthermore, preventive treatment should be capable of reversing, or at least reducing, the impact of CM on the cognition and QoL of migraineurs.

Methods

The cognition and QoL of 50 control subjects and 46 patients with CM were assessed using a battery of tests, prior to the use of preventive treatment based on botulinum toxin or oral drugs and after 3 months of this treatment.

Results

Compared with controls, patients with CM had lower scores on the assessment of cognitive performance (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure test [ROCF] (p<0.05), Trail Making Test [TMT] B) (p < 0.05) and QoL (p < 0.05). Three months after the use of preventive treatment, improvement was observed in all cognitive parameters (p < 0.05) and QoL (p < 0.05), except the ROCF copy task (p = 0.79). No statistically significant differences were observed when these outcomes were compared based on treatment.

Conclusions

This study confirms poor cognitive performance that is not explained by migraine pain itself, as it occurs in the interictal period, irrespective of the patient’s emotional status. Our findings show that these effects are reversible in some cases with preventive treatment of CM, reaffirming the important impact of this condition on the QoL of these patients, and the need to establish preventive treatment guidelines.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Chronic migraine (CM) is a complication of migraine that is defined by the presence of ≥ 15 headache days per month for at least 3 months, with at least 8 of those days meeting criteria for migraine, in the absence of medication overuse and other causes to which the headache could be attributed [1].

The estimated global prevalence of migraine is 14.0% [2]. It is the second leading cause of disability worldwide, and the first among young women [3]. It usually begins episodically, with the nervous system returning to a normal or premorbid functional state between attacks. However, approximately 2.5% of patients with episodic migraine experience transformation to CM [4]. The pathophysiology of CM and the mechanisms responsible for migraine chronification are not fully understood, although atypical pain processing during migraine episodes, central sensitization, cortical hyperexcitability, and neurogenic inflammation have been proposed as possible causes [3].

Management of CM consists of acute treatment of attacks with non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs or triptans, and preventive treatment to try to reduce the frequency, duration and intensity of attacks. Topiramate, beta-blockers, amitriptyline, and flunarizine are oral drugs commonly used as preventive agents in CM [5]. Within the injectable drug group, the use of botulinum toxin has been found to be beneficial in 75% of cases, and positive results have also been reported for anti-calcitonin gene-related peptide monoclonal antibodies in refractory cases [6, 7].

Patients with CM have more intense headaches and more severe symptoms than patients with episodic migraine, and the impact on their quality of life (QoL) is greater [8]. Some epidemiological studies have suggested that migraine is bidirectionally associated with several other disorders, including psychiatric disorders and chronic fatigue. It has also been associated with other pain-causing conditions, such as fibromyalgia, that are responsible for non-painful disabilities associated with migraine [9, 10]. Certain functional neurologic disorders, like psychogenic non-epileptic episodes and other functional movement-related disorders have also been linked to migraine and attempted suicides have also been reported amongst relatively young migraine patients [10, 11].

Migraine patients also frequently attend consultations for subjective cognitive complaints that are mainly attention- and memory-related and appear during or after migraine attacks, and contribute to the disability of patients experiencing them [12, 13]. Some factors related to CM, such as preventive therapies and comorbidities like depression, anxiety, and sleep disturbance, may contribute to the development of these cognitive deficits, although they are not the only cause of cognitive impairment in migraineurs [14, 15]. It has been suggested that certain biological mechanisms may be shared between migraine, fatigue, mood and cognition. Rather than true comorbidities, these mechanisms may be the reason for these associations. It is, however, currently unknown whether the treatment of migraine could influence the associated alterations [9,10,11]. Although several clinically based studies of patients with CM have described interictal impairment of cognitive function [16,17,18], results from studies based on other populations differ [19,20,21]. Longitudinal studies do not suggest any progressive decline in cognitive function [22, 23], while existing studies analysing cognitive performance in patients with CM have been generally limited in scope and number [15, 16, 24].

The functional organization of the neural networks associated with cognitive and pain processes may be altered by migraine at different levels, including pain processing and modulation. The impairment of structural network integrity increases with the presence of migraine attacks, indicating that CM causes the strongest structural abnormalities [25]. Affective, emotional and cognitive processing is also affected, specifically in the hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus and orbitofrontal cortex, and other areas in which differences have been identified on functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in CM patients [26,27,28,29,30]. These structural changes are already seen in patients with a migraine frequency of > 9 days per month [31]. Other cognitive domains affected in these patients include processing speed, attention span, memory, verbal skills, and executive function processes (working memory, divided attention, and planning) [32].

Despite evidence of the cognitive deficit associated with CM, few studies have been conducted in the context of cognitive impairments between attacks or on the effect of preventive treatment of CM on the reversibility of these impairments, emotional status and QoL. It is also still unclear whether the comorbidities present in patients with CM contribute indirectly to cognitive dysfunction or whether the underlying pathophysiological process of CM directly affects cognition. Given the high prevalence of migraine in the population, establishing an association between CM and cognitive decline could have substantial public health implications. Our hypotheses are that patients suffering from chronic migraine experience objective cognitive alterations that are not only due to the pain that they suffer or their current emotional state. Furthermore, preventive treatment should be capable of reversing, or at least reducing, the impact of CM on the cognition and QoL of migraineurs. With this in mind, the present study analyses impaired cognition in CM and the impact of this impairment on the QoL of CM patients before and after receiving preventive treatment based on oral therapies or botulinum toxin.

Methods

We conducted an observational study in the headache clinic at Hospital Universitari Arnau de Vilanova (Lleida, Spain) to analyse factors related to the cognitive status and QoL of patients with CM before receiving preventive treatment with botulinum toxin or oral drugs commonly used in the management of CM, and 3 months after receiving any of these therapies.

Study population

The study was approved by the ethics committee of Hospital Universitari Arnau de Vilanova (CEIC-2073), and informed consent was obtained from all participants. The cognitive status and QoL of 46 patients with CM and 50 healthy controls were evaluated. There were no significant differences in the age, sex or level of education of the subjects included in the groups. The CM patient group included individuals with normal brain MRI scans and CM diagnosed according to International Classification for Headache Disorders-III criteria [33]. Preventive treatment was initiated in these patients (treatment-naïve) or a second preventive drug was added if > 50% efficacy was not achieved with previous therapies. Patients with CM who had an oncological, inflammatory, or neurodegenerative disease and patients with decompensated psychiatric disease or who had experienced changes in the past 3 months if they were receiving psychiatric treatment were excluded (Supplementary Table S1).

Variables analysed

The factors analysed in this study were assessed in the interictal period so that acute pain did not interfere with the study results. We analysed general (age, sex, education level and work activity) and clinical variables (intensity and frequency of attacks pre- and post-treatment, age at onset of migraine, preventive treatment, acute treatment of attacks).

Variables related to mood, QoL, and cognition were also analysed. For mood, we used the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [HADS] [34], which is one of the scales most commonly used in trials to evaluate the combination of anxiety and depression, and also the Beck Depression Inventory-II [BDI-II] [35], which combines 21 items that measure the degree to which people feel sorry or guilty, lack energy, and note changes in their ability to do tasks in their real life. For QoL we used the 36-item Short Form Health Survey [SF36], which includes 36 items that are often used in trials, and also the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index [PSQI] [36, 37], to assess healthy sleep. For cognition, we used the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure test [ROCF] copy and reproduction task. We were therefore able to measure executive functions and visuospatial ability. With the second part of the test, we measured memory (ROCF memory task [ROCF-m] [38, 39] and used the Taylor Complex Figure Test for longitudinal evaluation [40] to avoid the learning effect. We then used the Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B [41] to measure attention and information processing speed (IPS). These tasks consist of finding ordered numbers on a sheet of paper and then, for the second part of the test, relating ordered numbers to letters of the alphabet which are also in order. This gave us a measure of working memory with which to make our evaluation.

A description of the sample and comparison with the controls was performed to evaluate the presence of cognitive impairments in the interictal period. Baseline (pre-treatment) and longitudinal (post-treatment) analyses were conducted to (i) analyse whether the cognitive impairment in CM are reversible after preventive treatment, (ii) determine the impact of these impairments on the QoL of CM patients, and (iii) study the differences in cognition and QoL of patients according to the drugs used for the treatment of CM.

To this end, descriptive analyses and comparison of the differences between the CM and control groups (Mann–Whitney U test) were performed at baseline. We used the Wilcoxon test to determine the changes resulting from the treatment at the longitudinal level.

Results

Sample description

The study included 46 CM patients, 40 of whom completed the study, and 50 controls of similar sex and age. The mean age of the sample was 47.17 years in the CM group and 47.82 years in the control group. No significant differences were found in the educational profile of the participants or in the duration of studies of the individuals included in each group (12.41 ± 4.76 years in the CM group and 13.72 ± 3.07 years in the control group). The measurement with respect to the time since onset of CM in the patients was 3.42 ± 4.34 years. Two-thirds of patients (67%) with CM were treatment-naïve, while the remaining 33% were already receiving oral preventive treatment at the time of inclusion in the study.

With regard to the preventive treatments used, 28.6% of patients used oral treatment only (beta-blockers, 50 mg/day; topiramate, 25 mg/12 h), 41.3% of cases used botulinum toxin only, and in 30.43% of cases, botulinum toxin was added to previous oral treatment (Table 1).

Table 1 Preventive treatments scheme (n = 46; 67% patients, treatment-naïve)

Regarding the response to preventive therapy after 3 months of treatment, a > 50% improvement was found in terms of a reduction in both the number of days and intensity of migraine in 79% of cases (p < 0.0001). No differences were found in the response rate in the treatment groups; the > 50% reduction in number of migraine days occurred in 85% of CM patients in whom oral preventive treatment was initiated, and in 83% of cases in which botulinum toxin was used.

Comparison between patients with CM and control subjects at baseline

Analysis of the differences between patients with CM and individuals in the control group was statistically significant for all the cognitive parameters analysed, QoL, and emotional status, with worse scores in the CM group (p < 0.0001) (Table 2, Figs. 1 and 2). No differences were observed between patients according to the drug used (p = 0.79).

Table 2 Comparison between the CM group (n = 46) and the control group (n = 50) in the cognition and quality of life scales used at baseline
Fig. 1
figure 1

Results of the ROCF copy task in the CM group and the control group. ROCF-c: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure test, copy task; CM: chronic migraine

Fig. 2
figure 2

Results of the QoL assessment in the CM group and the control group. Results were evaluated for the SF-36, HADS, PSQI, and BDI scales. QoL: quality of life; CM: chronic migraine; SF-36 PF: 36-item Short Form Health Survey, physical functioning; SF-36 RP: role-physical; SF-36 BP: bodily pain; SF-36 GH: general health; SF-36 VT: vitality; SF-36 SF: social functioning; SF-36 RE: role-emotional; SF-36 MH: mental health; BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory II; PSQI-GS: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, global score

Comparison between baseline and 3 months after treatment with preventive therapies in patients with CM

The differences in the results of the ROCF-m and TMT A tests after 3 months of treatment were statistically significant (p < 0.0001; p = 0.007), as were those comparing QoL (SF-36 bodily pain, p = 0.040; and SF-36 general health, p = 0.003); and PSQI, p = 0.025) before and after treatment (Table 3, Fig. 3). There was also a trend toward an improvement in depression (p = 0.052), although it did not reach statistical significance.

Table 3 Quality of life and cognition test scores before and three months after the start of preventive treatment in patients with CM
Fig. 3
figure 3

Results of the ROCF memory task in the CM group at baseline and 3 months after preventive treatment. ROCF-m: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure test, memory task

Comparison between patients with CM and control subjects after 3 months of treatment with preventive therapies in CM patients

To determine if CM patients who improved after receiving preventive therapies could achieve the level of cognitive performance of the controls, we examined cognition and QoL parameters in CM patients after 3 months of preventive treatment compared with controls. We found that after 3 months of preventive treatment, CM patients only reached normal values in the TMT A (p = 0.561), whereas the differences between the patients and the controls in the rest of the study variables remained statistically significant (Table 4, Fig. 4).

Table 4 Comparison between the CM group after three months of preventive treatment (n = 40) and the control group (n = 50) in the cognition and quality of life scales used
Fig. 4
figure 4

Results of the Taylor memory task in the CM group and the control group after 3 months of treatment with preventive therapies. Figure-Taylor-M-Long: Taylor Figure test, memory task (longitudinal); CM, chronic migraine

Furthermore, a group of 15 patients with cognitive impairment showed no significant improvement despite effective preventive treatment. We found that this lack of cognitive improvement was due not to a higher frequency or intensity of migraine, but to the greater presence of anxiety (p = 0.03) and depression (p = 0.02), which was associated with a lower level of education (p = 0.019).

Differences in cognitive performance between patients with CM and controls without mood alterations at baseline

We examined the impact of mood alterations on cognitive performance in CM patients. To this end, the sample of CM patients and controls was divided into those with normal mood, defined as scores < 9 on HADS and < 10 on BDI (n = 14), and cases with altered mood with higher scores (n = 32). The comparison between CM patients and controls without mood alterations revealed statistically significant differences in the ROCF-m test (p = 0.001), SF-36 score (p = 0.001), and in the variables analysed for depression (p = 0.004) and sleep (p < 0.05) (Table 5, Fig. 5).

Table 5 Comparison between patients with normal mood in the CM group (n = 14) and the control group (n = 50) at baseline
Fig. 5
figure 5

Results of the ROCF memory task in the CM group and the control group without mood changes at baseline. ROCF-m: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure test, memory task

Differences in cognitive performance based on patient mood before and after 3 months of preventive treatment

When comparing the pre- and post-preventive treatment results in the evaluation of cognition and QoL in the subgroup of patients with CM and normal mood, an improvement was observed in all the parameters analysed, although these differences were not statistically significant. In this case, we found that patients who showed improvement achieved the scores of the controls in the Taylor Figure Test for longitudinal evaluation (Table 6, Fig. 6).

Table 6 Results of the quality of life and cognition tests in the subgroup of patients with CM and normal mood at baseline (n = 14) and after three months of preventive treatment (n = 12)
Fig. 6
figure 6

Results of the assessment of the Taylor memory task in the subgroups of patients with CM and normal mood 3 months after preventive treatment and of control subjects with normal mood. Figure-Taylor-M-Long: Taylor Figure test, memory task (longitudinal); CM, chronic migraine

Comparison between patients with CM according to time since onset of migraine

Patients with CM were divided into 2 groups according to the time since onset of migraine (0–2 years, n = 23; > 2 years, n = 17). Although no statistically significant differences were observed in any of the variables analysed, patients with > 2 years since onset obtained better scores than those with migraine 0–2 years since onset (Table 7).

Table 7 Comparison of the parameters analysed in relation to cognition and quality of life in patients with CM with 0–2 years since onset of migraine (n = 23) or patients with CM with > 2 years since onset (n = 17)

Discussion

Summary of results

The present study compared patients with CM to age/sex matched controls and found that patients with CM had worse cognitive and QoL profiles that improved after 3 months of preventive treatment until reaching the same scores as the controls in the cases without mood alterations.

The patients with CM included in this study scored worse than subjects in the control group on all parameters analysed for physical health (SF-36), quality of sleep (PSQI), and anxiety and depression (BDI-II). These results are consistent with those of previous studies which concluded that patients with CM have a severe functional disability that is related to their psychiatric symptoms and pain [8, 17]. Moreover, some previous studies have described how depression and anxiety are not only comorbidities of CM, they also confer greater disability in CM patients [12, 13]. This correlates with the findings of this study that showed persistent cognitive impairment despite preventive treatment in patients with a higher rate of anxiety and depression and, in addition, in patients with a lower level of education. However, when we subdivided the study sample based on anxiety and depression parameters, we found that differences in cognition between CM patients and control subjects persisted in those with a normal emotional status. This suggests that the alterations in cognition are mainly caused by the pathophysiology of CM itself and, to a lesser extent, by its comorbidities.

This study found an association between CM and poor cognitive performance at the level of attention, visual memory, and executive functions, which is consistent with some previous studies in CM. Similar findings have been reported regarding poor performance in the attention and working memory of patients with CM during the interictal period, regardless of the use of topiramate or the presence of comorbidities [16]; similarly, impairments have been reported in problem-solving and decision-making [28] and in somatosensory temporal discrimination in these patients [42].

This analysis shows that 79% of the CM patients in the sample experienced an improvement in cognitive and QoL parameters, such as bodily pain, sleep quality and general health, after 3 months of treatment with preventive therapies. Although some studies have shown that this improvement may be independent of preventive treatment as it is also observed in non-responders [43], others conducted in CM patients with comorbidities treated with onabotulinumtoxin A also described improvements in the parameters of anxiety, depression, fatigue and sleep after the use of preventive therapies [44]. Specifically, Ho et al.reported an improvement in cognitive status, depression, and anxiety in a sample of 60 CM patients treated with botulinum toxin, although no relationship was found between the degree of improvement at 6–12 weeks after treatment and improvement in the memory, anxiety, and depression tests analyzed [43].

This study also showed a clear improvement in all the items analysed in relation to cognition, suggesting that preventive treatment improves deficits in attention, visual memory, and executive functions, although this improvement only reached normal scores in terms of attention in the overall group of patients with CM. Subgroup analyses however showed that preventive treatment did achieve normal ranges in patients with normal mood, suggesting that, although emotional changes alone do not explain the cognitive dysfunction in CM, they may interfere with the reversibility of these alterations during preventive treatment. In this specific case, attention is the parameter that correlates most with improvement in pain and is least influenced by the other comorbidities. This is consistent with a recent study that observed that patients with migraine, and especially those with allodynia, experience a decline in attentional inhibition during the interictal phase of migraine which is independent of confounding factors such as mood disorders [45].

The oral and injectable drugs used in this study have been associated with adverse effects that may be relevant when interpreting our results. One example is the negative impact of topiramate on the concentration ability of patients [46], an adverse effect that occurs in 2% of cases and takes place during the titration period, and in a lesser manner during the maintenance period [47]. In this study, no differences were observed with respect to the preventive treatment used in relation to the reversibility of cognitive impairments or QoL, which could be due to the fact that all patients who completed the study had good tolerance to the therapies used.

The effect of time since onset of CM on cognitive performance was also analysed. We observed that patients with longer time since onset of CM obtained better scores in all the studied items than those with shorter time, although these differences did not reach statistical significance. These results coincide with previous studies in which older patients with CM showed superior cognitive performance compared to control subjects [48, 49], as well as with longitudinal studies that showed no cognitive decline over time in migraineurs [22, 23]. These findings, including those reported here, support the theory of neuronal plasticity, whereby changes in the organization of the brain’s cognitive network could constitute an adaptive factor in migraine and interfere with cortical changes that allow multimodal sensory integration, reflecting adaptation to headache recurrence [50,51,52].

This study has some limitations, mainly the small size of the patient sample analysed and the need to use short tests for rapid assessment in the clinic, to avoid adding a component of patient mental fatigue. In addition, there is a limitation of the learning factor in the longitudinal assessment at 3 months of treatment, which we attempted to minimize by using Taylor Complex Figure Test for longitudinal evaluation at this point. Nevertheless, even considering these limitations, the brevity of the tests makes them more applicable and generalizable to clinical practice, and in turn indicates that the changes identified are more obvious, as they are detected with screening tests without requiring an extensive neuropsychological battery. Another limitation may have been the short duration of our follow-up, which may not have been long enough to detect any other appreciable differences. It would be beneficial to increase the sample size and also the duration of the follow-up period (from 6 to 12 months) in future studies.

Conclusions

This study confirms the cognitive impairment at the attention-perception level, divided attention, and in visual memory in patients with CM, impairments that are not explained only by the pain associated with the pathology, since they are observed in the interictal period. These impairments may show improvement in the post-treatment period following the use of preventive therapies, including better visual memory, attention, executive functions and, to a lesser extent, visual perception in patients with CM. This improvement reached the control cognition levels in patients without emotional alterations and correlates with a reduction in the impact on the QoL of migraineurs. Based on these results, modifying the initiation of a preventive treatment could reduce the interictal cognitive impairment related to CM. However, depression and anxiety could limit the reversibility of cognitive deficit. The implications of this observations are that a more routine assessment of cognitive deficits and emotional status could give a more accurate measure of cognitive performance in patients with CM and perhaps provide useful clues for finding a comprehensive treatment. Our findings confirm the need to implement effective preventive treatment early in cases of CM and to focus on their mood state.

Availability of data and materials

The data generated during this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Carod-Artal FJ, Irimia P (2012) Ezpeleta D [Chronic migraine: definition, epidemiology, risk factors and treatment]. Rev Neurol 54:629–637

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Stovner LJ, Hagen K, Linde M, Steiner TJ (2022) The global prevalence of headache: an update, with analysis of the influences of methodological factors on prevalence estimates. J Headache Pain 23(1):34

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Jensen R, Uluduz D, Katsarava Z, Lifting The Burden: the Global Campaign against H (2020) Migraine remains second among the world’s causes of disability, and first among young women: findings from GBD2019. J Headache Pain 21:137

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Schwedt TJ (2014) Chronic migraine. BMJ 348:g1416

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Aurora SK, Brin MF (2017) Chronic migraine: an update on physiology, imaging, and the mechanism of action of two available pharmacologic therapies. Headache 57:109–125

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Khan S, Olesen A, Ashina M (2019) CGRP, a target for preventive therapy in migraine and cluster headache: Systematic review of clinical data. Cephalalgia 39:374–389

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Lipton RB, Varon SF, Grosberg B et al (2011) OnabotulinumtoxinA improves quality of life and reduces impact of chronic migraine. Neurology 77:1465–1472

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Seng EK, Buse DC, Klepper JE et al (2017) Psychological factors associated with chronic migraine and severe migraine-related disability: an observational study in a tertiary headache center. Headache 57:593–604

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Karsan N, Goadsby PJ (2021) Migraine is more than just headache: is the link to chronic fatigue and mood disorders simply due to shared biological systems? Front Hum Neurosci 3(15):646692

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Pompili M, Serafini G, Di Cosimo D, Dominici G, Innamorati M, Lester D, Forte A, Girardi N, De Filippis S, Tatarelli R, Martelletti P (2010) Psychiatric comorbidity and suicide risk in patients with chronic migraine. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 7(6):81–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Ziplow J (2021) The psychiatric comorbidities of migraine in children and adolescents. Curr Pain Headache Rep 25(11):69

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Buse DC, Manack A, Serrano D, Turkel C, Lipton RB (2010) Sociodemographic and comorbidity profiles of chronic migraine and episodic migraine sufferers. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 81:428–432

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lanteri-Minet M, Radat F, Chautard MH, Lucas C (2005) Anxiety and depression associated with migraine: influence on migraine subjects’ disability and quality of life, and acute migraine management. Pain 118:319–326

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Gil-Gouveia R, Oliveira AG, Martins IP (2016) The impact of cognitive symptoms on migraine attack-related disability. Cephalalgia 36:422–430

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Santangelo G, Russo A, Trojano L et al (2016) Cognitive dysfunctions and psychological symptoms in migraine without aura: a cross-sectional study. J Headache Pain 17:76

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Ferreira KS, Teixeira CT, Cafaro C et al (2018) Chronic migraine patients show cognitive impairment in an extended neuropsychological assessment. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 76:582–587

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Lee SH, Kang Y, Cho SJ (2017) Subjective cognitive decline in patients with migraine and its relationship with depression, anxiety, and sleep quality. J Headache Pain 18:77

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Pellegrino Baena C, Goulart AC, Santos IS, Suemoto CK, Lotufo PA, Bensenor IJ (2018) Migraine and cognitive function: baseline findings from the Brazilian longitudinal study of adult health: ELSA-Brasil. Cephalalgia 38:1525–1534

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. George KM, Folsom AR, Sharrett AR et al (2020) Migraine headache and risk of dementia in the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. Headache 60:946–953

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Jiang W, Liang GH, Li JA, Yu P, Dong M (2022) Migraine and the risk of dementia: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Aging Clin Exp Res. 34(6):1237–1246

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Wang L, Wu JC, Wang FY, Chen X, Wang Y (2022) Meta-analysis of association between migraine and risk of dementia. Acta Neurol Scand 145:87–93

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gaist D, Pedersen L, Madsen C et al (2005) Long-term effects of migraine on cognitive function: a population-based study of Danish twins. Neurology 64:600–607

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Rist PM, Dufouil C, Glymour MM, Tzourio C, Kurth T (2011) Migraine and cognitive decline in the population-based EVA study. Cephalalgia 31:1291–1300

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Latysheva N, Filatova E, Osipova D, Danilov AB (2020) Cognitive impairment in chronic migraine: a cross-sectional study in a clinic-based sample. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 78:133–138

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Michels L, Koirala N, Groppa S, Luechinger R, Gantenbein AR, Sandor PS, Kollias S, Riederer F, Muthuraman M (2021) Structural brain network characteristics in patients with episodic and chronic migraine. J Headache Pain 22(1):8

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Gil-Gouveia R, Martins IP (2019) Cognition and Cognitive Impairment in Migraine. Curr Pain Headache Rep 23:84

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Gomez-Beldarrain M, Oroz I, Zapirain BG et al (2015) Right fronto-insular white matter tracts link cognitive reserve and pain in migraine patients. J Headache Pain 17:4

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Mongini F, Keller R, Deregibus A, Barbalonga E, Mongini T (2005) Frontal lobe dysfunction in patients with chronic migraine: a clinical-neuropsychological study. Psychiatry Res 133:101–106

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Schwedt TJ, Schlaggar BL, Mar S et al (2013) Atypical resting-state functional connectivity of affective pain regions in chronic migraine. Headache 53:737–751

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Zhang M, Liu Y, Hu G et al (2020) Cognitive impairment in a classical rat model of chronic migraine may be due to alterations in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunits. Mol Pain 16:1744806920959582

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Schwedt TJ, Chong CD, Wu T, Gaw N, Fu Y, Li J (2015) Accurate Classification of Chronic Migraine via Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Headache 55:762–777

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Vuralli D, Ayata C, Bolay H (2018) Cognitive dysfunction and migraine. J Headache Pain 19:109

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS) The International Classification of Headache Disorders, 3rd edition. Cephalalgia. 2018;38:1–211. https://doi.org/10.1177/0333102417738202

  34. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 67:361–370

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK (1996) Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX

    Google Scholar 

  36. (AIPS) AIdPdS (1997) Vigilia Sueño

    Google Scholar 

  37. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ (1989) The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 28:193–213

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Osterrieth PA (1944) Le test de copie d’une figure complexe; contribution a l’etude de la perception et de la memoire. Archives de psychologie. 30:206–356

    Google Scholar 

  39. Rey A (1941) L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie traumatique. (Les problems.). Archives de Psychologie 28:215–285

    Google Scholar 

  40. del Pino R, Peña J, Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Schretlen DJ, Ojeda N (2015) Taylor Complex Figure test: administration and correction according to a normalization and standardization process in Spanish population. Rev Neurol 61:395–404

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Bowie CR, Harvey PD (2006) Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nat Protoc 1:2277–2281

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Vuralli D, Evren Boran H, Cengiz B, Coskun O, Bolay H (2016) Chronic Migraine Is Associated With Sustained Elevation of Somatosensory Temporal Discrimination Thresholds. Headache 56:1439–1447

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Ho S, Darby D, Bear N (2020) Cognitive effects of onabotulinumtoxinA in chronic migraine. BMJ Neurol Open 2:e000014

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Boudreau GP, Grosberg BM, McAllister PJ, Lipton RB, Buse DC (2015) Prophylactic onabotulinumtoxin a in patients with chronic migraine and comorbid depression: an open-label, multicenter, pilot study of efficacy, safety and effect on headache-related disability, depression, and anxiety. Int J Gen Med 8:79–86

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Su M, Wang R, Dong Z, Zhao D, Yu S (2021) Decline in attentional inhibition among migraine patients: an event-related potential study using the Stroop task. J Headache Pain 22(1):34

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Hu C, Zhang Y, Tan G (2021) Advances in topiramate as prophylactic treatment for migraine. Brain Behav 11:e2290

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Adelman J, Freitag FG, Lainez M et al (2008) Analysis of safety and tolerability data obtained from over 1,500 patients receiving topiramate for migraine prevention in controlled trials. Pain Med 9:175–185

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Rist PM, Kang JH, Buring JE, Glymour MM, Grodstein F, Kurth T (2012) Migraine and cognitive decline among women: prospective cohort study. BMJ 345:e5027

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Wen K, Nguyen NT, Hofman A, Ikram MA, Franco OH (2016) Migraine is associated with better cognition in the middle-aged and elderly: the Rotterdam Study. Eur J Neurol 23:1510–1516

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Coppola G (2015) Neural plasticity and migraine. J Headache Pain 16:A26

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Lovati C, Giani L, Mele F et al (2016) Brain plasticity and migraine transformation: fMRI evidences. Expert Rev Neurother 16:1413–1425

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Tolner EA, Chen SP, Eikermann-Haerter K (2019) Current understanding of cortical structure and function in migraine. Cephalalgia 39:1683–1699

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Medical writing support was provided under the guidance of the authors by Javier Arranz-Nicolás, PhD, and Antonio Torres, PhD, from Medical Statistics Consulting (MSC), Valencia, Spain, in accordance with Good Publication Practice (GPP3) guidelines (Battisti, WP et al. Ann Intern Med. 2015).

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Funding

The authors disclose receipt financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors confirm that all authors listed: made a substantial contribution to the concept or design of the work; or acquisition, analysis or interpretation of data; drafted the article or revised it critically for important intellectual content; approved the version to be published; each author has participated sufficiently in the work to take public responsibility for appropriate portions of the content. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cristina González-Mingot.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted according to the ethical principles of the World Medical Associations Declaration of Helsinki. Ethics approval was obtained from the hospital’s ethics committee approval ID: (CEIC-2073) and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

The author gives the publisher license of the copyright. All authors guarantee that the research findings have not been previously published.

Competing interests

The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplementary Table S1.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

González-Mingot, C., Gil-Sánchez, A., Canudes-Solans, M. et al. Preventive treatment can reverse cognitive impairment in chronic migraine. J Headache Pain 23, 121 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s10194-022-01486-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s10194-022-01486-w