Skip to main content
  • Review Article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Placebo and other psychological interactions in headache treatment

Abstract

We present a theory according which a headache treatment acts through a specific biological effect (when it exists), a placebo effect linked to both expectancy and repetition of its administration (conditioning), and a non-specific psychological effect. The respective part of these components varies with the treatments and the clinical situations. During antiquity, suggestions and beliefs were the mainstays of headache treatment. The word placebo appeared at the beginning of the eighteenth century. Controversies about its effect came from an excessive interpretation due to methodological bias, inadequate consideration of the variation of the measure (regression to the mean) and of the natural course of the disease. Several powerful studies on placebo effect showed that the nature of the treatment, the associated announce, the patients’ expectancy, and the repetition of the procedures are of paramount importance. The placebo expectancy is associated with an activation of pre-frontal, anterior cingular, accumbens, and periacqueducal grey opioidergic neurons possibly triggered by the dopaminergic meso-limbic system. In randomized control trials, several arms design could theoretically give information concerning the respective part of the different component of the outcome and control the natural course of the disease. However, for migraine and tension type headache attacks treatment, no three arm (verum, placebo, and natural course) trial is available in the literature. Indirect evidence of a placebo effect in migraine attack treatment, comes from the high amplitude of the improvement observed in the placebo arms (28% of the patients). This figure is lower (6%) when using the harder criterium of pain free at 2 h. But these data disregard the effect of the natural course. For prophylactic treatment with oral medication, the trials performed in the last decades report an improvement in 21% of the patients in the placebo arms. However, in these studies the duration of administration was limited, the control of attacks uncertain as well as the evolution of the co-morbid psycho-pathology. Considering the reviews and meta-analysis of complex prophylactic procedures, it must be concluded that their effect is mostly linked to a placebo and non-specific psychological effects. Acupuncture may have a slight specific effect on tension type headache, but not on migraine. Manual therapy studies do not exhibit difference between manipulation, mobilization, and controls; touch has no proven specific effect. A comprehensive efficacy review of biofeedback studies concludes to a small specific effect on tension type headache but not on migraine. A review of behavioral treatment conclude to an interesting mean improvement but did not demonstrated a specific effect with the exception of a four arm study including a pseudo meditation control group. Expectation-linked placebo, conditioning, and non-specific psychological effects vary according clinical situations and psychological context; likely low in RCT, high after anempathic medical contact, and at its maximum with a desired charismatic healer. The announcements of doctors strongly influence the beliefs of patients, and in consequence their pain and anxiety sensibilities; this modulates the amplitude of the placebo and the non-specific psychological effects and is therefore a major determinant of the therapeutic success. Furthermore, any repetitive contact, even through a placebo, may interfere positively with the psychopathological co-morbidity. One has to keep in mind that the non-specific psychological interactions play a major role in the improvement of the majority of the headache sufferers.

Introduction

In pre-scientific civilizations, suggestions of the healer and the beliefs of the patients were the mainstays of headache treatment: in the Ebers papyrus [1], which was written in approximately 1200 BC, one treatment was to firmly bind a clay crocodile holding a grain in its mouth to the patient’s head with a strip of linen bearing the names of the gods. Another was to use a bandage with honey and coriander. In the dialogues of Plato (Charmides), approximately 400 BC, Socrates states that he used a leaf and a charm to treat headache.

The aim of this review is to determine from studies currently available in the PubMed database (US National Library of Medicine, National Daily Institutes of Health), how placebo and psychological effects take their place beside the biological specific action of treatments in the headache management. To assess this issue, we present successively a review of the basic studies on placebo effect, the contribution of the randomized control trials (RTC) in headache, and a sketch of a theory on headache treatment healing in clinical practice, with a special regard on the psychological interactions.

Some basic considerations concerning placebo

Interestingly, one had to wait until Hooper’s English medical dictionary in 1811 to see in the medical literature, the word “placebo”. It was used to designate a medicine given more to please than to treat (see [2]). The placebo is therefore, basically, an inert product which has to be compared to the active principle called verum; by extension, a complex therapeutic procedure has also to be compared to a sham (placebo) procedure. The scientific debate about placebo in therapeutic trials began in 1955 in a seminal article by Beecher [3] entitled “The powerful placebo”. This “power” remains controversial due to the concomitant confusion factors which have been comprehensively reviewed [4] (natural course of the disease, additional treatment, observer bias, irrelevant response, verum toxicity, patient bias, misquotation…) [57]. As we shall see below these misleading factors have to be controlled by an appropriated methodology. At the turn of the last century, a bio-psychological approach with several powerful studies shed light on the placebo phenomena [8].

Influence of what is offer to the patients

The color and number of pills influence the outcome [9, 10]: placebo tablets given to students and told to have a psychological effect act as stimulants when they are red and depressants when blue, and two placebo pills have a higher effect than one. The branding increases the placebo effect [11]. Parenteral or subcutaneous administration is more efficient than oral administration [12, 13]. A pseudo-acupuncture sham device had a greater effect than a placebo pill in chronic arm pain [14]. Finally, the more complex the procedure including rituals, mysterious powers, high technology and surgery, the larger the effects that are seen [13, 14].

The associated announcement is of paramount importance. A placebo cream presented as a powerful local anesthetic only induces an effect where it is applied [15]. The amplitude of the analgesic effect of saline given as a pain-killer after thoracic surgery depends on previously announced analgesic power [16]. In healthy young adults, an exercise program induces psychological well-being only in the group where this psychological effect has been announced and emphasized [17].

Practitioners’ attitude influences the placebo effect: in irritable bowel syndrome treated by pseudo-acupuncture, a warm empathic interaction enhances the placebo effect, but only in patients with an high extraversion profile [18].

Patients’ expectations and personality

The desire to be relieved, the expectations from the procedure, the memory of previous effects [19, 20], and the overt and covert meanings of the procedure [8] influence the placebo effect according to an expectation response theory in which expectations are the major determinant of what will occur in a given situation [21].

However, a relationship between the placebo reactivity and a given psychological or a socio-cultural state was not evidenced in a large study on patients with a persistent distal upper arm pain; the dimensions tested were: anxiety, depression, belief in alternative medicine, sex, color (white/non-white), educational level, and age [22].

Genuine placebo effect and confusion factors

The placebo effect linked to expectation is, therefore, a consequence of the idea of having received the verum. This genuine placebo effects have to be differentiated from the contingent events and from the natural course of the disease during the same period. In RCT, patients are often included when their symptoms worsen above a certain threshold. This threshold can be misleadingly reached when a fluctuating symptom is at its maximum or when it is erroneously measured in excess, thus a decrease or regression to the mean of the symptoms may have no biological signification (see review in [23]). Accordingly, a placebo effect can only be measured when comparing patients treated with placebo to non-treated patients during the same time period [7]. Even in this case, a bias can occur in the non-treated group due to the negative impact of lack of treatment.

Neurobiology of the placebo effects associated with expectations

The first step was the discovery of a link between expectation, placebo improvement and opioidergic mediation. Indeed, after molar extraction, naloxone, a μ-opioid receptor antagonist, reduced the beneficial effect of saline that was presented as an analgesic [24].

Pioneering work in PET and in fMRI showed that the expectation linked placebo analgesia was associated with increased activity in the rostral anterior cingulum [25, 26]. Using PET measurements of the fixation potential of 11C-carfentanil, a μ-opioid receptor ligand, several authors [27, 28] showed the implication of an opioidergic network involving pre-frontal and anterior cingular cortex, accumbens nuclei and peri-acqueducal gray in expectation linked placebo effect. The meso-limbic and orbito-frontal dopaminergic systems has also been demonstrated to be implicated by simultaneously measuring the fixation potential of 11C-raclopride, a D2/D3 agonist. The placebo-induced expectation is associated with a bilateral increase in dopaminergic activity in the ventral putamen and nucleus accumbens, which seem to play a trigger role in μ-opioidergic activation. Furthermore, it has been shown that an increase in pain sensation (i.e., a nocebo effect) is associated with opioid and dopaminergic deactivation [29]. Finally, μ-opioidergic and D2/D3 dopaminergic systems are activated to different degrees, depending on the patient’s positive (placebo) or negative (nocebo) expectation [30]. The analgesic placebo-induced expectation network is a part of a wider emotion control system [31].

Conditioning

A conditioning effect, not linked to expectations has been demonstrated. A study by Pavlov showed that, after coupling a tone (neutral stimulus) and apomorphine (unconditioned stimulus), the drug-induced symptoms still continue only after sound alone. There are several animal studies demonstrating that saline can induce an effect when replacing a drug given before repeatedly (see review in [32]). Such a conditioning has been demonstrated in humans, using the tourniquet paradigm (measuring daily the duration of hand contraction under ischemia); indeed ketorolac administered repeatedly reduced the pain sensation; then its substitution by a placebo, presented as an antibiotic was associated with the persistence of an analgesic effect; this effect persisted when the placebo was associated with naloxone [33]: this interesting experience demonstrates that in human, a conditioning may induce an analgesia which is not linked to an expectation, nor to an opioidergic mechanism.

When a treatment is given repeatedly, conditioning and expectation are intermingled, and the effect is reinforced with the length of the symptom-free period [34]. In an interesting study, conditioning was revealed to have a more powerful analgesic effect than expectation [32].

Lessons given by RCT

We assume that the outcome of headache management is the result of additive actions of (1) a specific effect on headache mechanisms, (2) a placebo effect linked to the idea of having received the verum, and (3) a non-specific psychological covert intervention (empathy, kind listening, etc…) which can be at its maximum in some complex therapeutic procedures as acupuncture, touch and manual therapy, biofeedback.

Therefore, to demonstrate a genuine placebo effect for an oral treatment, one should ideally use, at least, a three arm trial design (verum, placebo, and no treatment) [6]. Differences between the verum and placebo reflect the specific effect. Differences between the placebo and no treatment measure the placebo effect. To analyze the complex procedure effect one should, also, control the non-specific psychological covert intervention by the mean of a pertinent “psychological” control group. Bias may come from a non-convincing sham procedure, or from the negative effect of being included in a “psychological”, or in a non-treated control group.

At this point, it appears interesting to clarify the relationships between placebo, non specific psychological intervention and psychotherapy [35]. The three act through psychological processes. Placebo effect is simply mostly based on an expectation after the announcement of given therapy and after conditioning, the non-specific psychological intervention is limited to empathy, kind listening without psychological base, and psychotherapy acts mostly through specific intentionally delivered psychological interactions.

Acute treatment for headache attacks

No three arm trials have studied acute treatment [36]. In migraine, meta-analysis [3742] of placebo arms show that in adult patients, at 2 h, a two point improvement (using a 0–3 scale) is seen in about 28–29% of patients and a pain-free state in 6–9% (verum 58% for improvement and of 29% for pain-free), however, with a high heterogeneity [40]. These figures demonstrate the specific effect of the verum included in the meta-analysis, and their amplitude suggests that a genuine placebo effect does exist, mostly when a permissive outcome is chosen. A high placebo efficacy is reported in children: meta-analysis of 13 studies found that at 2 h after administration, improvement was seen in 33% (23–43%) of children, and a pain-free state in 14% (9–18%) [43].

A meta-analysis of 37 studies about the treatment of tension type headache attacks shows that NSAIDs and acetaminophen have a significant specific effect [44], but not data are available to analyze a putative placebo effect.

Prophylactic treatment and placebo effect

From a comprehensive meta-analysis of three arm trials [36], only five studied headache exclusively [4549]; unfortunately, none of these studies considered the actual IHS classification. Only one three arm trial concerned oral medication, given 2 weeks, and did not evidence for a difference in headache score between the placebo an no-treatment [47].

In a comprehensive meta-analysis of oral prophylactic treatments RCT of migraine (32 studies) [50], the percentage of patients presenting a 50% reduction in the number of days with headache reported in the placebo arms is 21% (13–28%). There was a significant heterogeneity. The corresponding data for the verum was 41% (33–49%). The improvement under placebo was greater in parallel compared to cross-over studies and in European compared to North American trials. These studies were performed between 1998 and 2004, usually lasted 12 weeks and did not mention the level of control of the acute attacks. A recent follow-up during 16 months of migraine patients with an optimized attack treatment [51] showed an improvement with time without differences between placebo alone, beta blockers alone, or placebo associated with behavioral management, suggesting that the administration of any treatment, even a placebo, is sufficient to achieve an apparent therapeutic success [51].

In conclusion, a specific effect of the prophylactic oral treatments included in the meta-analysis seems to be demonstrated [52] only for a relatively short time use. A prophylactic effect of placebo is also suggested [50] for a short time period by the amplitude of the improvement observed in the placebo arm. Finally, one study suggests a placebo prophylactic efficacy for a long period [51].

Meta-analysis of studies of tension type headache prophylaxis with oral treatment provide conflicting results: a lack of superiority of antidepressant medication or myorelaxants over placebo is reported in one study [53], and a beneficial effect of tricyclic antidepressants in two others [54, 55]. No data are presented to evaluate a putative placebo effect. Interestingly, the follow-up of four groups of patients with chronic tension-type headache [54] treated with anti-depressant medication or placebo with or without stress-management therapy, showed that the placebo had a non-different effect compared to the anti-depressant medication or stress-management therapy given alone on headache activity in the sub-group with initial low CTTH severity and on disability in the sub-group without initial mood and anxiety disorders [56]. This part of the data raises the issue of the placebo efficacy in CTTH of low severity.

The effects of acupuncture in migraine prevention have been evaluated by one meta-analysis [57]: true acupuncture was not superior to sham acupuncture, but is superior to no treatment up to 4 months after treatment (effect size 0.44 SD). In tension type headache, two meta-analysis [58, 59] revealed a small advantage of true acupuncture over sham acupuncture, in fact linked to one heavy positive study [60]. One have to conclude to a lack of specific effect of acupuncture on migraine and to a questionable specific effect of this procedure on tension-type headache. Acupuncture seems to act mostly through a high placebo and non-specific psychological effect.

A cervical pain trial meta-analysis studying manual therapy [61] found that manipulation (high velocity low amplitude) and mobilization produce similar effects on pain and are not better at short- and intermediate-term than controls for pain relief. Consequently, no specific and no significant placebo effects have been demonstrated. However, interpersonal touch has a major impact in our everyday social interactions [62], and has been used as a therapy since the dawn of humanity. Touch therapy is more or less codified (healing touch, therapeutic touch, Reiki) and is consistently associated with a special surrounding that can be considered as having a “non-specific” psychological influence. A meta-analysis of studies conducted on touch therapy for pain [63] includes only one inconclusive study [64] on tension type headache. Therefore, no conclusion about the specific effect of touch on headache can be drawn.

An interesting three arm study [49] on chronic headache sufferers treated by soft manual therapy with relaxation (Trager’s technique), controlled “attentional” visits, or no treatment shows a higher improvement of quality of life in the two treated groups compared to the no-treatment group. Consequently, no specific effect can be concluded from this open study. The improvement in the two groups may be due to the psychological non-specific effect.

Interestingly, the beneficial effect of sham acupuncture on headache has been proposed as a model of ritual healing by touch [65], which provides one way to explain the powerful efficacy of sham acupuncture when compared to no-treatment.

A comprehensive efficacy review of biofeedback (BFB) [66] concluded that true BFB is not significantly superior to sham BFB in migraine (effect size 0.25, confidence interval 95% 0.49–0.00) but did show a small advantage of true BIB over sham in tension-type headaches (effect size 0.50, confidence interval 95% 0.26–0.75). In both conditions, BFB is superior to the waiting list. Thus, BFB seems to have a specific beneficial effect on tension type headache. The superiority of pseudo BFB on the waiting list may be due to the additive effect of the non-specific psychological effect and the placebo effect of BFB.

In children, an interesting three arm study [48] (warming BFB associated with cognitive stress management therapy, pseudo BFB associated with an attention therapy, waiting-list) did not find evidence for significant inter-treatment difference and cannot conclude to a specific effect.

A review of the studies of behavioral treatment of headache [67] reports a 35–55% improvement but also emphasizes many methodological imperfections, including selection bias, credibility of the control procedure, and lack of reproducibility of the results. In addition, most of these studies were performed more than 30 years ago, and a control waiting list group was not reported. Among these studies, an interesting one [46] compares four treatments for tension headache (relaxation, relaxation + cognitive therapy, pseudo-meditation, and waiting list) and reports a significantly better improvement of a headache index for the two groups treated with relaxation compared to the pseudo-meditation group. Pseudo-meditation consisted of an equal number of sessions in which subjects were engaged in imaging daily activity without becoming relaxed, and is therefore a control of the relaxation. This study provides evidence for a specific effect of relaxation on tension type headache prophylaxis.

As conclusions from this review on RCT

A specific effect of treatment has been demonstrated by meta-analysis in several situation: (1) oral treatment of migraine, and tension type headache attacks, (2) oral treatment for migraine prevention during usually a 12-week administration, regardless the level of control of the attack and the underlying anxio-depressive state. In tension type headache, a questionable specific effect is also reported for acupuncture and for BFB associated with relaxation, and, by one study for relaxation.

A placebo effect is likely associated with every kind of treatment. However the evidences are only indirect. (1) In migraine attack, the amplitude of the improvement in the placebo arms (about half of that observed in the verum arms, if we disregard the improvement due to the natural course) replaces a demonstration. However if we consider the harder outcome of pain free at 2 h, this placebo effect is only about a fifth of that of verum. (2) In oral prophylaxis of migraine, the meta-analysis of short-term RCT reports also an improvement half of that of verum for placebo-treated patients, which is also an indirect proof of a short-term genuine placebo effect. Interestingly one study suggest a long-term placebo effect in chronic tension type headache in patients with a moderate disability or with a low initial anxio-depressive level, and in migraine patient with an optimal attack control.

Both placebo and non specific psychological effect are likely at the origin of the improvement induced by many procedures (migraine prophylaxis by acupuncture or biofeedback, headache in general for manual therapy, touch and behavioral treatment) on the evidences that for these techniques in these precise conditions, the patents improve though no specific effect has ever been demonstrated. The non- specific psychological effects of these complex procedures in headache treatments refer to the “common factors” shared by the various modalities of psychotherapies (see review in [68]

Towards a theory of the treatment for headache sufferers

Expectation-linked placebo, and non-specific psychological effects, and conditioning, vary according clinical situations and psychological context of the patients.

Variability of expectation linked placebo and conditioning effects (see also review [69])

The placebo effect linked to expectation is likely low in RCT as this situation does not favor a full effect of suggestion because of the formality of the inclusion, announcement of the side-effects and the known eventuality of receiving a non-active drug. Conversely, suggestions in clinical daily practice may be of great importance: a positive enthusiastic announcement of a beneficial effect will certainly have a better therapeutic effect than a restrictive announcement putting forward side-effects. One can formulate as a reasonable working hypothesis that the efficacy of some charismatic healers or shamans may be associated to massive μ-opioidergic/D2D3-dopaminergic mobilization. Furthermore, in prophylactic repetitive treatment, which is equivalent to a ritual, the effects of suggestion and conditioning are intermingled, likely reenforced by a long delay of occurrence, and a good control of the first attack [56]. Consequently, the more a prophylactic treatment worked at its beginning, the more it will continue working.

Influence of the psychological and psychopathological context

Repeated headaches induce negative affects with negative cognitive, affective (pain fear) and physiologic consequences, according to an individual dimension of “pain sensibility”. In return, negative effects can induce attacks, increase their intensity and the subsequent disability according to an individual “anxiety sensitivity” [70, 71] (Table 1; Fig. 1). Both “pain sensibility” and “anxiety sensibility” depend on beliefs, as placebo/nocebo phenomena depend on expectations. In daily clinical practice, the announcements of doctors strongly influence the expectations and beliefs of patients, which then influence the amplitude of placebo effect and of the pain and anxiety sensibilities, and are therefore a major determinant for a therapeutic success. There is an increase in psychopathological co-morbidity in chronic migraine and tension type headache [72, 73]. Consequently, any repetitive treatment, even a placebo, acting on this dimension may modify the natural course of the disease [74]. According to Frank [68], this beneficial effect may be due to the reduction of the “demoralization”, likely presented by the headache sufferers.

Table 1 Components of the therapeutic outcome of headache patients
Fig. 1
figure 1

Repeated headaches induce negative affects according to an individual dimension of “pain sensibility”. Negative affects increase repeated headaches according to an individual dimension of “anxiety sensitivity”. Both are modulated, as placebo/nocebo effects by beliefs and expectancies strongly influenced by doctors’ words

Conclusion

Headache is the last phase of activation of neuronal networks and can be powerfully controlled by analgesic and psychological systems. Suggestion from the outside, internal beliefs and expectations, and psycho-pathological context are deeply influenced by any therapeutic proposition. Inter-individual variations of these factors are potentially highly important. Consequently, when a patient trusts in a procedure, one has to accept the fact that this procedure is effective for him and produces corresponding biological consequences. Specific treatments act effectively on the common final mechanisms of migraine or tension headache in combination with the other non-specific factor.

References

  1. Klein Karl H (1905) Von The medical features of the papyrus Ebers, Chicago, American medical association. http://archive.org

  2. de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Schulzer M, Stoessi J (2002) The placebo effect in neurological disorders. Lancet Neurol 1:85–91, 12849512, 10.1016/S1474-4422(02)00038-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Beecher HK (1955) The powerful placebo. J Am Med Assoc 159:1602–1606, 13271123, 10.1001/jama.1955.02960340022006, 1:STN:280:DyaG28%2FitlWntw%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Kienle GS, Kiene H (1997) The powerful placebo effect: fact or fiction. J Clin epidemiol 50:1311–1318, 9449934, 10.1016/S0895-4356(97)00203-5, 1:STN:280:DyaK1c7htFGrtQ%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (2001) Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment. N Engl J Med 344:1594–1602, 11372012, 10.1056/NEJM200105243442106

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Krogsbøll LT, Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (2009) Spontaneous improvement in randomised clinical trials: meta-analysis of three-armed trials comparing no treatment, placebo and active intervention. BMC Med Res Methodol 9:1, 19123933, 10.1186/1471-2288-9-1

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (2010) Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 20:CD003974

    Google Scholar 

  8. Moerman D, Jonas W (2002) Deconstructing the placebo effect and finding the meaning response. Ann Inter Med 136:471–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Blackwell B, Bloomfield SS, Buncher CR (1972) Demonstration to medical students of placebo responses and non-drug factors. Lancet 10:1279–1282, 10.1016/S0140-6736(72)90996-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. de Craen A, Roos P, de Vries A, Kleijnen J (1996) Effect of colour of drugs: systematic review of perceived effect of drugs and of their effectiveness. BMJ 313:1624–1626, 8991013, 10.1136/bmj.313.7072.1624

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Brandhwaite A, Cooper P (1981) Analgesic effect of branding in treatment of headache. Br Med J 282:1576–1578, 10.1136/bmj.282.6276.1576

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. de Crean A, Tijssen J, de Gans J, Kleijnen J (2000) Placebo effect in acute treatment of migraine: subcutaneous placebos are better than oral placebos. J Neurol 247:183–188, 10.1007/s004150050560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Kaptchuk TJ, Stason WB, Davis RB, Legedza AR, Schnyer RN, Kerr CE, Stone DA, Nam BH, Kirsch I, Goldman RH (2006) Sham device v inert pill: randomised controlled trial of two placebo treatments. BMJ 18:391–397, 10.1136/bmj.38726.603310.55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Kaptchuck T, Goldman P, Stone D, Stason W (2000) Do medical devices have enhanced Placebo effects? J Clin Epidemiol 53:786–792, 10.1016/S0895-4356(00)00206-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Benedetti F, Arduino C, Amanzio M (1999) Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by target-directed expectations of analgesia. J Neurosci 19:3639–3648, 10212322, 1:CAS:528:DyaK1MXislGrsb0%3D

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Pollo A, Amanzio M, Arslanian A, Casadio C, Maggi G, Benedetti F (2001) Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance. Pain 93:77–84, 11406341, 10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00296-2, 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3MzktlCksw%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Desharnais R, Jobin J, Cote C, Levesque, Godin G (1993) Aerobic exercise and the placebo effect: a controlled study. Psychosom Med 55:149–154

  18. Kelley JM, Lembo AJ, Ablon JS, Villanueva JJ, Conboy LA, Levy R, Marci CD, Kerr CE, Kirsch I, Jacobson EE, Riess H, Kaptchuk TJ (2009) Patient and practitioner influences on the placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome. Psychosom Med 71:789–797, 19661195, 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181acee12

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Price DD, Milling LS, Kirsch I, Duff A, Montgomery GH, Nicholls SS (1999) An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm. Pain 83:147–156, 10534585, 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00081-0, 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3c%2Fgs1WmtQ%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Benedetti F, Pollo A, Lopiano L, Lanotte L, Vigetti S, Rainero I (2003) Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. J Neurosci 23:4315–4323, 12764120, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3sXktFKnsrk%3D

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kirsch I (1985) Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior. Am Psychol 40:1189–1202, 10.1037/0003-066X.40.11.1189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Bertisch SM, Legedza AR, Phillips RS, Davis RB, Stason WB, Goldman RH, Kaptchuk TJ (2009) The impact of psychological factors on placebo responses in a randomized controlled trial comparing sham device to dummy pill. J Eval Clin Pract 15:14–19, 19239577, 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2008.00942.x

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Barnett AG, van der Pols JC, Dobson AJ (2005) Regression to the mean: what it is and how to deal with it. Int J Epidemiol 34:215–220, 15333621, 10.1093/ije/dyh299

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL (1978) The mechanism of placebo analgesia. Lancet 23:654–657, 10.1016/S0140-6736(78)92762-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M (2002) Placebo and opioid analgesia-imaging a shared neuronal network. Science 295:1737–1740, 11834781, 10.1126/science.1067176, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD38XhslehsLo%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD (2004) Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 303:1126–1127, 10.1126/science.1093065

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Zubieta J-K, Bueller J, Jackson L, Scott D, Yanjun X, Koeppe R, Nichols T, Stholer C (2005) Placebo effects mediated by endogenous opioid activity on mu-opioid receptors. J Neurosci 25:7754–7762, 16120776, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0439-05.2005, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD2MXpvVKgu78%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Wager TD, Scott DJ, Zubieta JK (2007) Placebo effects on human mu-opioid activity during pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:11056–11061, 17578917, 10.1073/pnas.0702413104, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD2sXnsV2ks7Y%3D

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK (2008) Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opiod and dopaminergic responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:230–231, 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Zubieta JK, Stohler CS (2009) Neurobiological mechanisms of placebo responses. Ann NY Acad Sci 1156:198–210, 19338509, 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04424.x, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD1MXlt1GitLo%3D

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Petrovic P, Dietrich T, Fransson P, Andersson J, Carlsson C, Ingvar M (2005) Placebo in emotional processing-induced expectations of anxiety relief activate a generalized modulatory network. Neuron 46:957–969, 15953423, 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.023, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD2MXlvVWjtbo%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Vaudouris N, Peck C, Colemen G (1990) The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain 43:121–128, 10.1016/0304-3959(90)90057-K

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Amanzio M, Benedetti F (1999) Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. J Neurosci 19:484–494, 9870976, 1:CAS:528:DyaK1MXltVSquw%3D%3D

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Finnis D, Kaptchuck T, Miller F, Benedetti F (2010) Biological, clinical, and ethical advances in placebo effects. Lancet 375:686–695, 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61706-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Kirsh I (2005) Placebo psychotherapy: synonym or oxymoron. J Clin Psychol 61:791–801, 10.1002/jclp.20126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (2010) Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 20:CD003974

    Google Scholar 

  37. Ferrari M, Roon K, Lipton R, Goadsby P (2001) Oral triptans in acute migraine treatment: a meta-analysis of 53 trials. Lancet 358:1668–1675, 11728541, 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)06711-3, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3MXos1ajsro%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Oldman A, Lesley A, McQuay H, Moe A (2002) Pharmacological treatments for acute migraine: quantitative systematic review. Pain 97:247–257, 12044621, 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00024-6, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD38XktVartrw%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Bendtsen L, Mattsson P, Zwart JA, Lipton RB (2003) Placebo response in clinical randomized trials of analgesics in migraine. Cephalalgia 23:487–490, 12950372, 10.1046/j.1468-2982.2003.00528.x, 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3svns1Ghsg%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Loder E, Goldstein R, Biondi D (2005) Placebo effects in oral triptan trials: the scientific and ethical rationale for continued use of placebo controls. Cephalalgia 25:124–131, 15658949, 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2004.00817.x, 1:STN:280:DC%2BD2M%2Fjt1KksQ%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Macedo A, Farre M, Banos JE (2006) A meta-analysis of the placebo response in acute migraine and how this response may be influenced by some of the characteristics of the clinical trials. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 62:161–172, 16402240, 10.1007/s00228-005-0088-5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Diener HC, Schorn CF, Bingel U, Dodick DW (2008) The importance of placebo in headache research. Cephalagia 28:1003–1011, 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2008.01660.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Fernandes R, Ferreira JJ, Sampaio C (2008) The placebo response in studies of acute migraine. J Pediatr 152:527–533, 18346509, 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.09.024

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Verghagen A, Damen l, Berger M, Lenssinck L, Passchier J, Kroes J (2010) Treatment of tension type headache: paracetamol and NSAIDs work : a systematic review. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd 154:A1924

  45. Classen W (1984) Placebo application, personality, and headaches: a signal detection theory analysis of experimentally induced pain in comparison to clinical pain. Pharmacopsychiatry 17:98–101, 6377336, 10.1055/s-2007-1017416, 1:STN:280:DyaL2c3kvVamsw%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Blanchard E, Appelbaum K, Radnitz C, Mitchultka D, Morill B, Kirsh C, Hillhouse J, Evans D, Guarnieri G, Attanasio V, Andrasik F, Jaccard J, Dentinger M (1990) Placebo-controlled evaluation of abbreviated progressive muscle relaxation and relaxation combined with cognitive therapy in the treatment of tension headache. J Consult Clin Psychol 8:210–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Blanchard E, Appelbaum K, Radnitz C, Morill B, Mitchultka D, Kirsh C, Guarnieri G, Hillhouse J, Evans D, Attanasio V, Jaccard J, Barron K (1990) A clinical evaluation of thermal biofeedback and thermal biofeedback combined with cognitive therapy in the treatment of vascular headache. J Consult Clin Psychol 58:216–224, 2186067, 10.1037/0022-006X.58.2.216, 1:STN:280:DyaK3c3ktl2huw%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Scharff L, Marcus D, Masek BA (2002) Controlled of minimal-contact thermal biofeedback treatment in children with migraine. J Pediatr Psychol 27:106–119, 10.1093/jpepsy/27.2.109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Foster K, Liskin J, Cen S, Abott A, Armisen V, Globe D, Knox L, Mitchchell M, Azen S (2004) The Trager approach in the treatment of chronic headaches: a pilot study. Altern Ther 10:40–45

    Google Scholar 

  50. Macedo A, Banos JE, Farre M (2008) Placebo response in the prophylaxis of migraine: a meta-analysis. Eur J Pain 12:68–75, 17451980, 10.1016/j.ejpain.2007.03.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Holroyd KA, Cottrell CK, O’Donnell FJ, Cordingley GE, Drew JB, Carlson BW, Himawan L (2010) Effect of preventive (beta blocker) treatment, behavioural migraine management, or their combination on outcomes of optimised acute treatment in frequent migraine: randomised controlled trial. BMJ 29:c4871. doi:10.1136/bmj.c4871, 10.1136/bmj.c4871

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Fumal A, Schoenen J (2008) Tension-type headache: current research and clinical management. Lancet Neurol 7:70–83, 18093564, 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70325-3, 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD1cXhtlaqtbw%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Verhagen A, Damen L, Berger M, Passchier J, Koes B (2010) Lack of benefit for prophylactic drugs of tension-type headache in adults: a systematic review. Fam Pract 27:151–165, 20028727, 10.1093/fampra/cmp089

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Moja PL, Cusi C, Sterzi RR, Canepari C (2005) Selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs) for preventing migraine and tension-type headaches. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 20:CD002919

    Google Scholar 

  55. Jackson JL, Shimeall W, Sessums L, Dezee KJ, Becher D, Diemer M, Berbano E, O’Malley PG (2010) Tricyclic antidepressants and headaches: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 20(341):c5222. doi:10.1136/bmj.c5222, 10.1136/bmj.c5222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Holroyd K, Labus J, Carlson B (2009) Moderation and modulation in the psychological and drug treatment of chronic tension-type headache: the role of disorder severity and psychiatric co-morbidity. Pain 143:213–222, 19342174, 10.1016/j.pain.2009.02.019

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Linde K, Allais G, Brinkhaus B, Manheimer E, Vickers A, White AR (2009) Acupuncture for migraine prophylaxis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 21:CD001218

    Google Scholar 

  58. Linde K, Allais G, Brinkhaus B, Manheimer E, Vickers A, White AR (2009) Acupuncture for tension-type headache. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 21:CD007587

    Google Scholar 

  59. Davis M, Konowech R, Rolin S, Spiering E (2008) Acupuncture for tension-type headache: a meta-analysis of randomized trials. J Pain 9:667–677, 18499526, 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.03.011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Endres H, Bowing G, Diener H, Lange S, Maier C, Molsberger A, Zenz M, Vickers A, Tegenthoff M (2007) Acupuncture for tension-type headache. J Headache Pain 8:306–314, 17955168, 10.1007/s10194-007-0416-5

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Gross A, Miller J, D’Sylva J, Burnie SJ, Goldsmith CH, Graham N, Haines T, Brønfort G, Hoving JL (2010) COG. Manipulation or mobilisation for neck pain: a Cochrane Review. Man Ther 15:315–333, 20510644, 10.1016/j.math.2010.04.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Gallace A, Spence C (2010) The science of interpersonal touch: an overview. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:246–259, 18992276, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. So PS, Jiang Y, Qin Y (2008) Touch therapies for pain relief in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 8:CD006535

    Google Scholar 

  64. Keller E, Bzdek V (1986) Effect of therapeutic touch on tension headache pain. Nurs Res 35:101–106, 3633503, 1:STN:280:DyaL287kvVGnsg%3D%3D

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Kerr CE, Shaw JR, Conboy LA, Kelley JM, Jacobson E, Kaptchuk TJ (2011) Placebo acupuncture as a form of ritual touch healing: a neurophenomenological model. Conscious Cogn 20:784–791, 21397519, 10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.009

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Nestoriuc Y, Martin A, Rief W, Andrasik F (2008) Biofeedback treatment for headache disorders: a comprehensive efficacy review. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 33:125–140, 18726688, 10.1007/s10484-008-9060-3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Rains J, Penzien D (2005) Behavioral research and the double blind placebo-controlled methodology: challenges in applying the biomedical standard to behavioral headache research. Headache 45:479–486, 15953264, 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2005.05099.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Frank JD Frank JB (1993) Persuasion and healing: a comparative study of psychotherapy, 3rd edn. The John Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, p 292

  69. Antonaci F, Chimento P, Diener HC, Sances G, Bono G (2007) Lessons from placebo effects in migraine treatment. J Headache Pain 8:63–66, 17361384, 10.1007/s10194-007-0360-4

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Nicholson R, Houle T, Rhudy J, Norton P (2007) Psychological risk factors in headache. Headache 47:413–426, 17371358

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Janssen SA (2002) Negative affect and sensitization to pain. Scand J Psychol 43:131–137, 12004950, 10.1111/1467-9450.00278

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Autret A, Roux S, Rimbaux-Lepage S, Valade D, Debiais S (2010) Psychopathology and quality of life burden in chronic daily headache: influence of migraine symptoms. J Headache Pain 11:247–253, 20383733, 10.1007/s10194-010-0208-1, 1:STN:280:DC%2BC3czitVGhuw%3D%3D

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Radat F, Swendsen J (2005) Psychiatric comorbidity in migraine: a review. Cephalalgia 25:165–178, 15689191, 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2004.00839.x, 1:STN:280:DC%2BD2M%2FmtVyqsw%3D%3D

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Heckman BD, Holroyd KA, Himawan L, O’Donnell FJ, Tietjen G, Utley C, Stillman M (2009) Do psychiatric comorbidities influence headache treatment outcomes? Results of a naturalistic longitudinal treatment study. Pain 146:56–64, 19660866, 10.1016/j.pain.2009.06.019

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Conflict of interest

None.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. Debiais.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 2.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Autret, A., Valade, D. & Debiais, S. Placebo and other psychological interactions in headache treatment. J Headache Pain 13, 191–198 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10194-012-0422-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10194-012-0422-0

Keywords